\documentclass[11pt]{article} \usepackage{geometry} % See geometry.pdf to learn the layout options. There are lots. \geometry{letterpaper} % ... or a4paper or a5paper or ... %\geometry{landscape} % Activate for for rotated page geometry \usepackage[parfill]{parskip} % Activate to begin paragraphs with an empty line rather than an indent \usepackage{graphicx} \usepackage{amssymb,amsmath} \usepackage{verbatim} \usepackage{hyperref} % Should be imported after all other packages! See http://en.wikibooks.org/wiki/LaTeX/Hyperlinks \hypersetup{colorlinks=true, linkcolor= red, % color of internal links citecolor= blue, % color of links to bibliography filecolor=green, % color of file links urlcolor=green % color of external links } \begin{document} \begin{center} \vspace{1cm} \Large The Predominance of Wild-Animal Suffering\\ over Happiness: An Open Problem \vspace{.8cm} \end{center} \begin{abstract} ``The total amount of suffering per year in the natural world is beyond all decent contemplation," to quote Richard Dawkins \cite{dawkins}. In particular, I find it plausible that the aggregate amount of pain endured by wild animals exceeds the aggregate amount of happiness, especially in view of the fact that most offspring die soon after birth. If small creatures like insects, other arthropods, and perhaps even zooplankton can suffer, this conclusion seems especially likely, since these organisms live very short lives before dying in often-painful ways, and they preponderate over larger animals by many orders of magnitude. %I present evidence supporting the position that wild animals, taken as a whole, experience more suffering than happiness. This seems particularly true if, as is possible if not very likely, insects\footnote{Throughout this piece, I use ``insects" as shorthand for ``higher-level arthropods," including spiders, millipedes, crabs, and others, in addition to members of the class \textit{Insecta}.} can suffer, since insects have lifespans of only a few weeks and are orders of magnitude more preponderate than other animals. What can be done about the problem? Probably the best we can do right now is promote concern about the issue among animal-welfare activists, academics, and the general public. This will help spur research on important topics like whether insects feel pain, what are the day-to-day hedonic experiences of animals in the wild, and what practical steps can be taken to alleviate their suffering (whether or not their lives involve, on balance, more pain than pleasure). If and when such approaches to improving wild-animal welfare become available, a larger base of concern about the issue will allow for faster and more widespread adoption. In addition, if people are more cognizant of the extraordinary acts of cruelty that occur daily in nature, they will give sober consideration to the consequences of possible future technological endeavors like terraforming, directed panspermia, or even creating new universes. See ``The Importance of Wild-Animal Suffering" (\url{http://www.utilitarian-essays.com/suffering-nature.html}) for further details and references. \end{abstract} \normalsize \section{Introduction}\label{Introduction} \begin{quote} The fact that in nature one creature may cause pain to another, and even deal with it instinctively in the most cruel way, is a harsh mystery that weighs upon us as long as we live. One who has reached the point where he does not suffer ever again because of this has ceased to be a man. ---Albert Schweitzer \cite[Ch. 3]{albert} \end{quote} Many advocates of animal welfare assume that animals in the wild are, on the whole, happy. For instance, the site utilitarian.org includes a proposal to create a wildlife reserve, the main ``Utility value" of which would come from ``The value of life over death for the animals who will live there" \cite{reserve}. The assumption of wild-animal happiness is also implicit in the arguments that animal welfarists make against interfering with nature. When asked whether we should prevent lions from eating gazelles, Peter Singer replied: \begin{quote} [\ldots] for practical purposes I am fairly sure, judging from man's past record of attempts to mold nature to his own aims, that we would be more likely to increase the net amount of animal suffering if we interfered with wildlife, than to decrease it. Lions play a role in the ecology of their habitat, and we cannot be sure what the long-term consequences would be if we were to prevent them from killing gazelles. [\ldots] So, in practice, I would definitely say that wildlife should be left alone. \cite{singer} \end{quote} Jennifer Everett mentions a similar idea: \begin{quote} [\ldots] if propagation of the ``fittest" genes contributes to the integrity of both predator and prey species, which is good for the predator/prey balance in the ecosystem, which in turn is good for the organisms living in it, and so on, then the very ecological relationships that holistic environmentalists regard as intrinsically valuable will be valued by animal welfarists because they conduce ultimately, albeit indirectly and via complex causal chains, to the well-being of individual animals. \cite[p. 48]{bambi} \end{quote} But is it true that wild animal lives are worth living? In rest of this piece, I present arguments against this view. Note, however, that even if wild-animal lives \textit{are}, on balance, positive, it may still be worthwhile to undertake efforts to alleviate their suffering. \section{Life and Death in the Wild} \subsection{A Neglected Topic} \begin{quote} Preoccupied with the [\ldots] task of countering those forms of more-than-human, and potentially avoidable, suffering that have their genesis not in nature, but in society, ecophilosophers and ecopolitical theorists have tended to be silent on the dire realities of pain, predation, disease and death that seem to be inseparable from organismic life in general. ---Kate Rigby, ``Minding (about) Matter: On the Eros and Anguish of Earthly Encounter" \cite{ecopreview} \end{quote} \begin{quote} Many humans look at nature from an aesthetic perspective and think in terms of biodiversity and the health of ecosystems, but forget that the animals that inhabit these ecosystems are individuals and have their own needs. Disease, starvation, predation, ostracism, and sexual frustration are endemic in so-called healthy ecosystems. The great taboo in the animal rights movement is that most suffering is due to natural causes. ---Albert, in Nick Bostrom's ``Golden" \cite{golden} \end{quote} \begin{quote} People who are appalled by the indiscriminate killing of wildlife by mechanisms such as leg-hold traps should recognize that the pain and suffering caused by cat predation is not dissimilar and the impacts of cat predation dwarf the impacts of trapping. ---Audubon Society of Portland \cite{cats} \end{quote} \begin{quote} It is often assumed that wild animals live in a kind of natural paradise and that it is only the appearance and intervention of human agencies that bring about suffering. This essentially Rousseauian view is at odds with the wealth of information derived from field studies of animal populations. Scarcity of food and water, predation, disease and intraspecific aggression are some of the factors which have been identified as normal parts of a wild environment which cause suffering in wild animals on a regular basis. ---``Captivity and Suffering," \emph{UCLA Animal Care and Use Training Manual} \cite{lab} \end{quote} \subsection{Are Animals Less Sensitive?} Alfred Russel Wallace wrote in 1910: \begin{quote} [\ldots S]maller birds and mammals [\ldots] are all so wonderfully adjusted to their environments, that, in a state of nature, they can hardly suffer at all from what we term accidents. Birds, mice, squirrels, and the like, do not get limbs broken by falls, as we do. They learn so quickly and certainly not to go beyond their powers in climbing, jumping, or flying, that they are probably never injured except by rare natural causes, such as lightning, hail, forest-fires, etc., or by fighting among themselves; and those who are injured without being killed by these various causes form such a minute fraction of the whole as to be reasonably negligible. The wounds received in fighting seem to be rarely serious, and the rapidity with which such wounds heal in a state of nature shows that whatever pain exists is not long-continued. [\ldots] Our whole tendency to transfer our sensations of pain to all other animals is grossly misleading. The probability is, that there is as great a gap between man and the lower animals in sensitiveness to pain as there is in their intellectual and moral faculties; and as a concomitant of those higher faculties. We require to be more sensitive to pain because of our bare skin with no protective armour or thick pads of hair to ward off blows, or to guard against scratches and wounds from the many spiny or prickly plants that abound in every part of the world; [\ldots] it is this specially developed sensibility that we, most illogically, transfer to the animal-world in our wholly exaggerated and often quite mistaken views as to the cruelty of nature! \cite[p. 376, 377, 379]{wallace} \end{quote} I do not pretend to give a full refutation of Wallace's naive claim, but consider the following point. It may provide insight into why Wallace and some of his contemporaries concluded that animals are less sensitive. \begin{enumerate} \item Sick and injured members of a prey species are the easiest to catch, so predators have evolved such that they deliberately target these individuals. \item As a consequence, those prey that appear sick or injured will be the ones killed most often. Thus, evolutionary pressure pushes prey species to avoid drawing attention to their suffering and to pretend as though nothing is wrong \cite[Ch. 4.12]{nuffield}. \item Humans may interpret this ``apparent stoicism" as evidence that animals are relatively insensitive to pain \cite{nuffield2}. However, this inference has been shown to be false \cite{nuffield2}. \end{enumerate} \subsection{Hunger, Thirst, and Cold} \begin{itemize} \item Many animals die of thirst during times of drought \cite{hindu}. \item ``Starvation and malnutrition occur in several wildlife species and routinely eliminates the young, old, weak, and sick animals. [\ldots] Historically, in Michigan the number of species diagnosed at the Laboratory as dying from malnutrition and starvation are second only to those dying from traumatic injuries" \cite{mal}. \item Stages of starvation \cite{mal}: \begin{enumerate} \item Within a few hours, carbohydrate glycogen in the liver is used up. \item Next, fat deposits are consumed. \item Finally, the body resorts to protein stored in the cytoplasm of cells. \item Eventually, the cells stop functioning. Too little glucose reaches the brain, triggering shock followed by death \cite{mal}. \end{enumerate} \item Hunger is particularly severe during the winter, due to greater energy requirements and the reduced availability of food \cite{mal}. Deer are sometimes unable to reach shoots buried below ice \cite{cold1}, and adult deer can lose 25\% to 30\% of their body weight over a severe winter \cite{mal}. During a particularly long period of cold in the Czech Republic in 2006, some deer blinded themselves by eating rape root, the only green plant they could find \cite{cold3}. \item 2006 was also a harsh year on bats in Placerville, California: \begin{quote} ``You can see their ribs, their backbones, and (the area) where the intestine and the stomach are is completely sunk through to the back," said Dharma Webber, founder of the California Native Bat Conservancy. [\ldots] She said emerging mosquitoes aren't enough to feed the creatures. ``It would be like us eating a little piece of popcorn here or there," she said. \cite{cold2} \end{quote} \item ``Birds unable to find a sheltered perch during the storm may have their feet frozen to a branch or their wings covered in ice making them unable to fly. Grouse buried in snow drifts are often encased by the ice layer and suffocate" \cite{cold1}. \end{itemize} \subsection{Examples of Diseases and Parasites} \textbf{Roundworms:} These parasites are some of the most common in the world \cite{worms2}. While generally innocuous, they can sometimes cause hemorrhages, tissue damage, and pneumonia---especially in young or infirm animals \cite{worms2}. \textbf{Stomach worm:} This parasite inhabits many common birds, including the crow, blue jay, and American robin \cite{worm1}. Stomach worm can make its hosts lethargic (hence, more easily caught by predators) and can reduce their immune defenses against other diseases. Additionally, if the worms build up too much ``thick, white, slimy mucous and sloughed stomach epithelial tissue" while enlarging part of the stomach, they can entirely block the entrance of food---causing the host to starve to death \cite{worm1}. \textbf{Rabies:} This disease is found on every continent except Australia and Antarctica \cite{disease1}. It's 100\% fatal and has highly erratic symptoms. An animal with ``dumb rabies" simply becomes paralyzed and succumbs to death. One with ``furious rabies" becomes excited and violent for a few days, after which it develops tremors, convulsions, paralysis, and then dies \cite{disease1}. \textbf{Salmonellosis:} Caused by \textit{Salmonella} bacteria, this disease affects fish, reptiles, amphibians, birds, and mammals all over the world \cite{disease2}. In birds, \begin{quote} Signs range from sudden death to gradual onset of depression over 1 to 3 days, accompanied by huddling of the birds, fluffed-up feathers, unsteadiness, shivering, loss of appetite, markedly increased or absence of thirst, rapid loss of weight, accelerated respiration and watery yellow, green or blood-tinged droppings. The vent feathers become matted with excreta, the eyes begin to close and, immediately before death, some birds show apparent blindness, incoordination, staggering, tremors, convulsions or other nervous signs. \cite{disease2} \end{quote} \textbf{Verminous Hemorrhagic Ulcerative Enteritis:} In waterfoul, the disease produces ``weakness, limber neck, wing droop, listlessness, lack of fear of humans, anorexia, and blood stained feathers around the vent" \cite{disease3}. Ingestion of as few as 20 trematode ``flukes" will cause death, usually within 3-8 days. Flukes attach to intestinal mucous and produce hemorrhagic ulcers; severe blood loss often kills the birds as a result of shock \cite{disease3}. \subsection{Predation} \begin{quote} Accidents and predators take some. Only occasional accidents cause quick relatively painless death. [F]requently severe bruising and bone breakage results from accidents; pain certainly accompanies this. Predators may or may not be efficient. Often hide and flesh may be torn or displaced, prey may be killed or escape with several wounds to die later or recover. ---``Hunting: Kindness or Cruelty?" \cite{humanehunt} \end{quote} \begin{quote} To believe in the carnivorous reptiles of geologic times is hard for our imagination---they seem too much like mere museum specimens. Yet there is no tooth in any one of those museum-skulls that did not daily through long years of the foretime hold fast to the body struggling in despair of some fated living victim. Forms of horror just as dreadful to the victims, if on a smaller spatial scale, fill the world about us to-day. Here on our very hearths and in our gardens the infernal cat plays with the panting mouse, or holds the hot bird fluttering in her jaws. Crocodiles and rattlesnakes and pythons are at this moment vessels of life as real as we are; their loathsome existence fills every minute of every day that drags its length along; and whenever they or other wild beasts clutch their living prey, the deadly horror which an agitated melancholiac feels is the literally right reaction on the situation. ---William James, \emph{The Varieties of Religious Experience} \cite[pp. 161-62]{james} \end{quote} In the following table, all information in a given row came from the source in the far-right column unless I include a citation elsewhere in the table, in which case only the cited portion came from a different source. Some entries in the table reference notes that appear subsequently. Note 1 is referenced by (1), etc. \footnotesize \begin{center} \begin{tabular}{|c|c|c|c|c|} \hline Predator & Examples of Prey & Means of killing & Duration of&Source\\ &&&killing (minutes)&\\ \hline lions&wildebeest, zebra, ungulates&suffocation (1)&5-6 \cite[p. 13]{raptor}&\cite{lion}\\ \hline cheetahs&gazelles&suffocation&often 5-15,&\cite{cheetah}\\ &&(occasionally 2-3 times)&sometimes 25&\\ \hline spotted hyenas&ungulates&biting off chunks of flesh&1 to 13&\cite{kruuk}\\ \hline wild dogs&ungulates&disembowelment (2)& &\cite[p. 18-22]{raptor}\\ \hline coyotes&rabbits, rodents, reptiles,&may stalk prey for 20-30&&\cite{coyote}\\ &hibians, fish&minutes until prey is exhausted&&\\ \hline heron&fish&swallow whole&$\leq 2$ to swallow&\cite{heron}\\ \hline piranha&fish, cattle, ``anything"&tear apart while alive&&\cite{piranha}\\ \hline constrictor snakes&rodents (3), \ mammals&prevent prey from inhaling (4)&1 \cite{snakey}&\cite{flank}\\ \hline small snakes&fish, frogs&swallow and digest alive (5)&&\cite{flank}\\ \hline venomous snakes&rodents, birds, reptiles&internal bleeding, paralysis (6)&a few minutes&\cite[p. 49]{raptor}\\ \hline crocodiles&birds, fish, reptiles, ungulates&grabbing in jaws, drowning (7)&&\cite[p. 43]{raptor}\\ \hline \end{tabular} \end{center} \normalsize \paragraph{Notes:} \begin{enumerate} \item \cite[p. 12-13]{raptor} vividly describes the capture of a zebra by a lioness: \begin{quote} The lioness sinks her scimitar talons into the zebra's rump. They rip through the tough hide and anchor deep into the muscle. The startled animal lets out a loud bellow as its body hits the ground. An instant later the lioness releases her claws from its buttocks and sinks her teeth into the zebra's throat, choking off the sound of terror. Her canine teeth are long and sharp, but an animal as large as a zebra has a massive neck, with a thick layer of muscle beneath the skin, so although the teeth puncture the hide they are too short to reach any major blood vessels. She must therefore kill the zebra by asphyxiation, clamping her powerful jaws around its trachea (windpipe), cutting off the air to its lungs. It is a slow death. If this had been a small animal, say a Thompson's gazelle (\textit{Gazella thomsoni}) the size of a large dog, she would have bitten it through the nape of the neck; her canine teeth would then have probably crushed the vertebrae or the base of the skull, causing instant death. As it is, the zebra's death throes will last five or six minutes. \end{quote} \item Describing a fictional wild-dog hunt, \cite[p. 22]{raptor} writes: \begin{quote} Two dogs converge on the [wildebeest] calf, causing it to veer away from the other wildebeests, still accompanied by its mother. The two wildebeests are still galloping at a fair speed, but the calf is beginning to tire. Three other dogs fall in behind the two pursuers. The lead dog closes with the calf and launches itself at one of its hind legs. Its sharp teeth make contact, and the jaws lock tight, bringing the calf to a faltering stop. A second dog grabs it by the nose. Two more tear at its underbelly, ripping it open and pulling out its gut. Its helpless mother looks on. \end{quote} \item ``Live mice will fight for their lives when they are seized, and will bite, kick and scratch for as long as they can" \cite{flank}. \item ``Small (and maybe large) constrictors seem to kill their prey by causing high blood pressure, heart attacks and strokes before suffocation" \cite{boa}. \item ``The snake drenches the prey with saliva and eventually pulls it into the esophagus. From there, it uses its muscles to simultaneously crush the food and push it deeper into the digestive tract, where it is broken down for nutrients" \cite{howstuff}. Prey animals often do not die immediately after being swallowed; this is illustrated by the fact that some poisonous newts, after being swallowed, use their toxins to kill their captor snake so that they can crawl back out of its mouth \cite[p. 59]{raptor}. \item \cite[p. 50]{raptor} tells of a colleague who was bitten by a rattlesnake: ``The pain, which he likened to holding his hand on a hot stove, lasted for four days." \item Another fictional narrative from \cite[p. 43]{raptor}: \begin{quote} A herd of wildebeests approaches a placid lake in the middle of a hot afternoon. [\ldots] Suddenly one of the crocodiles [in the lake] explodes from the water, clamping its massive jaws on the first thing it contacts. Bluntly pointed teeth splinter through bone, locking fast on a startled wildebeest's leg. The impact throws the wildebeest onto its side in the shallows. [\ldots] The crocodile gives an enormous tug, dragging the wildebeest into deeper water. With hooves threshing and eyes popping, it lets out a terrified bellow. The next instant its head is jerked beneath the surface. Choking water sears down its throat and floods its lungs. The wildebeest struggles violently to get its head above the water but does not stand a chance against such brute strength. \end{quote} \end{enumerate} \subsubsection{Endorphins:} In some cases, death by predation may not be quite as bad as it seems. Upon capture by predators, some animals release endorphins---the same chemicals that our brains produce when we exercise \cite[Question 2]{thinktank}. Endorphins can have a pronounced effect: \begin{quote} LORD SOULSBY: [\ldots I]s there any other situation where we can at least get evidence of death being either very painful, animal welfare or not---and I am talking about other predator situations, when it is known endorphins reach a very high level just at the end of a hunt when gazelle are attacked by a large cat? Is that a realistic argument to use that, in fact, in the prey predator situations that might also occur? MR WISE: Lord Soulsby, [\ldots] I have in front of me an article from a \emph{Sunday Telegraph} a couple of years ago. It is entitled, ``I was eaten by a hyena." It relates to a woman who suffered extremely serious injuries when she went into a cage in a wildlife centre. What she said was: ``The mind, I found, is strange. It shut off during the attack while my body continued to act without thought or even sight. I do not remember him sinking his teeth into my arm, although I heard, the little grating noise as his teeth chewed into the bone. Everything was black and slow and exploding in my stomach. Vision returned gradually, like an ancient black and white television," et cetera, et cetera. It goes on. Throughout this account, that is two pages, she constantly reiterates that she never experienced any pain whatsoever. Interestingly, she experienced anxiety, but not to the extent of her wound. But her sandals were pulled off during the attack, and she was worrying about mundane everyday things like the fact she would burn her feet on the sand. At the same time she was being destroyed by this hyena, which was an extraordinary thing. She also heard herself screaming, but was quite unaware of having screamed. \cite{british} \end{quote} \subsubsection{Fear of Predators} Not all predation attempts are successful. When hunting alone, lions capture a meal only 14\% to 32\% of the time, although this figure doubles when they hunt in groups of at least two \cite{schaller}. Hunting dogs and cheetahs capture prey roughly 70\% of the time \cite{schaller}. Since predators are not always successful, some prey endure the experience of fleeing from a predator even before the end of their lives. I didn't find literature that discussed how stressful these experiences might be, but I did find a study in which researchers traumatized mice by putting them in the presence of a cat \cite{ptsd}. The exposed mice exhibited short- and long-term ``behavioral and neurochemical changes consisting of acute increased anxiety associated with posttraumatic stress disorders" that mirrored the DSM-IV criteria \cite[p. 1-3]{ptsd}. \subsection{Lifespans} \begin{center} \begin{tabular}{|l|c|c|} \hline Type of Animal & Lifespan (years)\\ \hline Deer & 10 \cite{deer}\\ Squirrels & 6 \cite{squirrel}\\ Cats & 4 \cite{cat}\\ Raccoons & 3.5 \cite{raccoon}\\ Reptiles and amphibians & $\approx$ 3 \cite{bullfrog, snake}\\ Opossums & 1.5 \cite{opossum}\\ Dogs & 1.5 \cite{dogs}\\ Birds & 1 to 2 \cite{lack, birds}\\ Rats & 1 \cite{rats}\\ \hline \end{tabular} \end{center} \section{A Bug's Life} There seems generally to be a negative correlation between an animal's height on the ecological food pyramid and its level of welfare: Smaller prey animals tend to have shorter lifespans and more offspring that will not survive to maturity. Unfortunately, there is also a negative association between an animal's height on the pyramid and its preponderance, meaning that the animals that suffer more tend to have larger populations. Near the bottom of the animal food pyramid are insects (which I define to include spiders, crabs, and other higher-level arthropods). Judging from their short lifespans, these animals---if they are sentient---probably have some the worst lives of all. \begin{center} \begin{tabular}{|l|c|} \hline Type of Insect & Lifespan (weeks)\\ \hline Horn fly & 2 \cite{i4}\\ Trichogramma Wasps & 2 \cite{i2}\\ Stable fly & 3 \cite{i4}\\ Black fly & $\approx 3 \text{ to } \approx20$ \cite{i3}\\ Mosquito & $3 \text{ to } \approx150$ \cite{i3}\\ Damselflies & 3 to 4 \cite{i5}\\ \textit{Drosophila} & 6 \cite{i1}\\ Dragonflies & 6 to 8 \cite{i5}\\ Green lacewings & 12 \cite{i2}\\ Ladybird beetles & 48 \cite{i2}\\ \hline \end{tabular} \end{center} The sources that I used for this table were not specific about whether the ``average lifespans" that they reported included the large number of insects that die shortly after birth. I imagine that most of these figures only give the average lifespan of those individuals that make it past the first few days or weeks of age. Thus, these figures probably severely \textit{understate} how soon after birth most insects die. \subsection{But Are Insects Sentient?} I review the matter in more detail in another piece, ``Do Insects Feel Pain?"\footnote{\url{http://www.utilitarian-essays.com/insect-pain.html}} Some suggest we ``give insects the benefit of the doubt" as far as whether they can suffer. Giving insects the benefit of the doubt is a far more radical approach than I propose. Rather, I favor coming up with a rough subjective probability that insects can feel pain and then discounting the potential suffering that we cause them by that factor. I think a probability of 0.1 for insect sentience is conservative.\footnote{In Shakespeare's \emph{Measure for Measure}, Isabella tells Claudio: \begin{quote} The poor beetle, that we tread upon In corporal sufferance finds a pang as great As when a giant dies. \cite[Act 3, Scene 1]{shakespearebug} \end{quote} I am making a weaker claim. I assert merely that, on balance of expectation, a giant's death entails as much suffering as stepping on 10 beetles.} At any given moment, the earth supports $10^{18}$ insects \cite{somany}. This number becomes $10^{17}$ when we discount by a 0.1 probability of sentience. \subsection{Insect Predation} Unfortunately, insects endure many of the same hardships as higher animals, including predation and parasitism. Some insects become trapped in spider webs. Venemous spiders bite their prey, wrap them in silk, and dissolve them in digestive fluid \cite{spiders}. Even less fortunate are victims of the Amazonian \textit{Allomerus decemarticulatus} worker-ants, which build a trap out of plant fibers to catch passers-by \cite{torturerack}. The device has been dubbed a ``torture rack" because captured insects are stretched out on the fibers while they wait to be paralyzed by a sting from the ants. Some larger victims stay caught in the trap for up to 12 hours. Fortunately, only one other related species is known to build such traps \cite{torturerack}. The ichneumon wasps are a group of insect parasites that feed on their hosts in a particularly gruesome way. Sometimes called the ``farmer's friends," these creatures are used as biological control agents because of their success at eating caterpillars and larvae of pest insects \cite{farmerfriend}. Stephen Jay Gould describes the process by which some ichneumon wasps parasitize their hosts: \begin{quote} The free-flying females locate an appropriate host and then convert it into a food factory for their own young. Parasitologists speak of ectoparasitism when the uninvited guest lives on the surface of its host, and endoparasitism when the parasite dwells within. Among endoparasitic ichneumons, adult females pierce the host with their ovipositor and deposit eggs within. (The ovipositor, a thin tube extending backward from the wasp's rear end, may be many times as long as the body itself.) Usually, the host is not otherwise inconvenienced for the moment, at least until the eggs hatch and the ichneumon larvae begin their grim work of interior excavation. Among ectoparasites, however, many females lay their eggs directly upon the host's body. Since an active host would easily dislodge the egg, the ichneumon mother often simultaneously injects a toxin that paralyzes the caterpillar or other victim. The paralysis may be permanent, and the caterpillar lies, alive but immobile, with the agent of its future destruction secure on its belly. The egg hatches, the helpless caterpillar twitches, the wasp larvae pierces and begins its grisly feast. Since a dead and decaying caterpillar will do the wasp larvae no good, it eats in a pattern that cannot help but recall, in our inappropriate anthropocentric interpretation, the ancient English penalty for treason � drawing and quartering, with its explicit object of extracting as much torment as possible by keeping the victim alive and sentient. As the king's executioner drew out and burned his client's entrails, so does the ichneumon larvae eat fat bodies and digestive organs first, keeping the caterpillar alive by preserving intact the essential heart and central nervous system. Finally, the larvae completes its work and kills its victim, leaving behind the caterpillar's empty shell. \cite{gould} \end{quote} Unlike the relatively rare torture-rack ants, the ichneumonoidea are very common. This made the phenomenon of live parasitism especially troubling for natural theologians, as Gould notes: \begin{quote} The ichneumonoidea are a group of wasps, not flies, that include more species than all the vertebrates combined (wasp, with ants and bees, constitute the order Hymenoptera; flies, with their two wings---wasps have four---form the order Diptera). In addition, many non-ichneumonoid wasps of similar habits were often cited for the same grisly details. Thus, the famous story did not merely implicate a single aberrant species (perhaps a perverse leakage from Satan's realm), but hundreds of thousands---a large chunk of what could only be God's creation. \cite{gould} \end{quote} \section{An Energy-Cost Argument} In his 1995 paper ``Towards Welfare Biology," Yew-Kwang Ng constructs a theoretical description of animal motivation, a theorem of which is that the total amount of suffering in the wild exceeds the total amount of happiness \cite{ng}. I attempt a restatement of his argument in this section, though I do not know if my presentation correctly interprets Ng's original ideas. I welcome comments and corrections. \textbf{Assumption 1:} For the sake of simplicity, suppose that an organism's life can have one of two possible outcomes \begin{itemize} \item ``Winning": the organism survives, mates successfully, and has children, or \item ``Losing": the organism dies at a young age and fails to mate. \end{itemize} \textbf{Definition:} Consider a given species. Let $n$ be the ratio of the number of organisms of that species that ``lose" to the number of organisms of that species that ``win." For instance, if 2 organisms win and 4 lose, $n = \frac{4}{2} = 2$. \textbf{Assumption 2:} In terms of energy use and tissue maintenance, it's more costly for the body to produce states of strong emotion than states of hedonic neutrality \cite[p. 271]{ng}. The stronger the emotion, the greater the cost. \textbf{Definition:} Let $c$ be a real number representing the magnitude of cost that an organism expends in producing an emotional state. Define $c = 0$ for the cost of producing a regular equilibrium emotional state. Define $E(c)$ to be the magnitude of enjoyment that results when cost $c$ is applied toward producing happiness. Let $S(c)$ represent the corresponding magnitude for suffering. Even though suffering is a ``negative" emotion, let the magnitude of $S$ be $\geq 0$. \textbf{Assumption 3:} $E$ and $S$ are both differentiable functions of $c$ and both have the same shape. \textbf{Assumption 4:} As cost increases, it takes more and more cost to produce the same magnitude of change in emotional experience. In other words, $E$ and $S$ are concave. \textbf{Assumption 5:} The hedonic value of being in a state of relative equilibrium is essentially neutral; \textit{i.e.}, $E(0) = 0$ and $S(0) = 0$ \cite[p. 271]{ng}. \textbf{Definition:} Let $w$ be the value of $c$ such that $E(w)$ represents the enjoyment associated with ``winning." Let $\ell$ be the value of $c$ such that $S(\ell)$ represents the suffering associated with ``losing." The difference in the utility of winning and losing is then $E(w) + S(\ell)$. \textbf{Assumption 6:} Organisms are motivated to survive and reproduce because they seek the happiness of winning and hope to avoid the suffering of losing. In particular, the bigger is $E(w) + S(\ell)$, the more motivated organisms will be. \textbf{Assumption 7:} Organisms with greater motivation are more likely to survive. Hence, in the long run, organisms will develop hedonic mechanisms that maximize $E(w) + S(\ell)$ subject to the constraint of having a fixed amount of resources available for producing strong emotional states. \textbf{Result:} These assumptions are sufficient to establish what Ng calls the ``Buddhist premise"---that for species with $n > 1$, the overall amount of suffering is greater than the overall amount of happiness \cite[p. 272]{ng}. \textbf{Illustration:} To take an example, let $k > 0$ be a constant, and suppose that $E(c) = S(c) = k\ln(c+1)$ for $c \geq 0$. As Ng suggests \cite[p. 282]{ng}, this seems consistent with the Weber-Fechner law in psychology, which states that the perceived magnitude of a stimulus is a logarithmic function of its actual magnitude \cite{weberlaw}. Assumption 7 tells us to maximize $E(w) + S(\ell)$ subject to the constraint $bw + nb\ell = $ constant, where $b$ is the number of winning organisms in the species \cite[p. 277]{ng}. This gives the solution $w = n\ell + n - 1$.\footnote{To see this, use the method of Lagrange multipliers. Maximize $f(w, \ell) := E(w) + S(\ell) = k\ln(w+1) + k\ln(\ell+1)$ subject to $g(w, \ell) := bw + nb\ell = \text{constant}$. For some $\lambda$, \begin{equation*} \begin{split} \nabla f &= \lambda \nabla g\\ \Bigl(\frac{k}{w+1}, \frac{k}{\ell+1}\Bigr) &= \lambda(b, nb)\\ \end{split} \end{equation*} so that \begin{equation*} \begin{split} w+1 = \frac{k}{\lambda b} \text{ \ and \ }& \ell + 1 = \frac{1}{n}\frac{k}{\lambda b}\\ w+1 = n&(\ell + 1)\\ w = n&\ell + n-1.\\ \end{split} \end{equation*} } If $\ell$ equals some amount $\ell_0$, so that $w = n\ell_0 + n -1$, what is the balance of suffering to happiness? \begin{equation*}\label{log} \begin{split} nbS(\ell_0) - bE(n\ell_0 + n -1) &= nbk\ln(\ell_0+1) - bk\ln(n\ell_0 + n)\\ &= bk\Bigl[n\ln(\ell_0+1) - \ln n - \ln(\ell_0 + 1)\Bigr]\\ &= bk\Bigl[(n-1)\ln(\ell_0+1) - \ln n\Bigr].\\ \end{split} \end{equation*} Assuming $b = k = \ell_0 = 1$, this equation reduces to \begin{equation*}\label{log2} \text{amount by which suffering exceeds happiness} = (n-1)\ln 2 - \ln n, \end{equation*} which I plot in the following figure. \begin{figure} \centering \includegraphics[scale=.6]{log-big-nums.png} \label{fig1} \end{figure} The labels in these figures are roughly based off of data from \cite[p. 37]{popbook}, which Ng includes in a table in his piece \cite[p. 270]{ng}. These data gave typical values for the number of offspring produced by a female during a mating season. For instance, the bullfrog (\textit{Rana catesbeiana}) lays between 6,000 and 20,000 eggs per brood. To incorporate this number into Figure 2, I did the following: \begin{itemize} \item Take the average of the range: 13,000. \item Assume (conservatively) that a bullfrog has only one mating cycle in its lifetime. Hence, a single female produces on average 13,000 eggs. \item In a stable population, two of those eggs, on average, will survive to maturity, while the rest will die early. Hence, $n = \frac{13,000 - 2}{2} \approx 6,500$. \item In using this figure, I'm implicitly assuming that all of the other 13,000 - 2 organisms fell into the ``failure" group. If the reader finds this unrealistic, he or she may use a larger number of successes---say 50. Then $n = \frac{13,000 - 50}{50} = 259$, which is still considerable. \item I'm also implicitly assuming that all of the 13,000 eggs develop into a form capable of feeling pain before they die. In fact, many of the eggs are probably eaten before they hatch, while others fail to hatch altogether; if the eggs themselves feel no pain, this does not cause suffering. \end{itemize} \section{Emotional and Cognitive Biases} In this section, I suggest some biases that may affect our assessments of wild-animal suffering. \subsection{Availability Heuristic} The \textit{availability heuristic} refers to the tendency of people to overestimate the probability or prevalence of an outcome or feature of the world that they can readily imagine \cite{availability}. For instance, people often assess the risks of airplane crashes, natural disasters, shark attacks, and terrorism \cite{availability} as higher than they actually are because mental images of these events are more available than images of, say, workplace injury. The images of wild animals most readily available to many people are probably those from movies, zoos, and backyard birdfeeders. Even when people walk through a forest, they remark about the song of birds, the croaking of frogs, and the chirping of katydids. They fail to notice the thousands of arthropods upon which they are stepping\footnote{An article called ``Basic Facts: Insect Numbers" reports:\begin{quote}A person steps upon thousands of insects whenever walking outdoors. In an oak forest in Pennsylvania, researchers counted the number of arthropods in leaf litter and soil in samples that were one foot square and three inches deep. They found an average of 9,759 arthropods per square foot. Based on these counts, the researchers estimated that there were more than 425 million soil and litter arthropods per acre (43,560 square feet) of forest. [\ldots] In this study, mites (Class Arachnida) were the most abundant kind of animal, averaging more than 294 million per acre. Springtails (Class Hexapoda, Order Collembola), which are closely related to insects, averaged 119 million. Insects and other arthropods averaged 11 million per acre. \cite{insectnumbers} \end{quote}} or the sick and dying frogs and birds that suffer in quiet misery. \subsection{Selection Bias} \textit{Selection bias} happens when a researcher draws conclusions about a population on the basis of a statistically unrepresentative sample \cite{selection}. In this context, I intend the term to refer to a slightly different concept: the idea that, as is often said, ``History is written by the winners." Or in other words, the organisms who study wild animals and appraise their welfare are usually well-off human beings (not sick or marginalized members of human society, nor rabbits, tadpoles, or flies). Many people in Western societies have never been tortured, have never been chased by predators, and have never gone without food, water, and warmth for days on end. Even if we were (hypothetically) to ask other animals in the wild for their opinions, we would still be sampling ``the winners" of the group; we would miss out on the large numbers of babies that died shortly after being born. Selection bias also applies to emotional states. If you were in a state of severe agony, you wouldn't be reading this paper right now. Most of your important moral and practical decisions you probably make in a relatively ``euthymic state"---one in which you have your basic needs satisfied and feel mildly content. This is probably a good thing, since severe pain makes us irrational.\footnote{In George Orwell's \textit{1984}, O'Brien makes this point in its most extreme form: \begin{quote} [\ldots F]or everyone there is something unendurable---something that cannot be contemplated. Courage and cowardice are not involved. If you are falling from a height it is not cowardly to clutch at a rope. If you have come up from deep water it is not cowardly to fill your lungs with air. It is merely an instinct which cannot be destroyed. \cite{1984} \end{quote}} But it is important that we do not become complacent to suffering. We musn't blindly assume that other organisms share our emotional state, and we must remind ourselves of what it's like to suffer. \subsection{Wishful Thinking} \textit{Wishful thinking} refers to the tendency of people to predict better outcomes than rationality would dictate \cite{wishful}. Related is the fallacy of \textit{rosy retrospection}, in which people tend to remember well the better parts of an experience (such as a vacation) and ignore the irritating or unpleasant aspects of it \cite{rosy}. At the same time, depressed people may be more down to earth. At least, that's the hypothesis of \emph{depressive realism}, which claims that people with depression tend to have more accurate perceptions than happy people, especially as regards their own abilities \cite{depressive}. The types of psychological mechanisms described above seem generally helpful in allowing people to succeed in society, since those who don't dwell on negative thoughts will presumably be more motivated, productive, and healthy. Of course, this analysis is slightly complicated by the fact that positive delusions themselves actually make people happier, so that those who think that their lives are better \textit{actually do} have better lives, \textit{ceteris paribus}. But when we are examining the amount of suffering experienced by wild animals, we should aim for accuracy, rather than the conclusion that will make us feel good about the world. The sick gazelle that lies helpless on the ground, waiting to be torn apart by predators, is not helped by our desire to paint rosy Potemkin villages of nature. \subsection{Reverence for Nature} People feel a wonderful sense of awe when they spend time in nature. \begin{quote} [\ldots] He is insensibly subdued\\ To settled quiet: he is one by whom\\ All effort seems forgotten; one to whom\\ Long patience hath such mild composure given,\\ That patience now doth seem a thing of which\\ He hath no need. He is by nature led\\ To peace [\ldots] \cite{word} \end{quote} Thomas Huxley remarked: \begin{quote} The vast and varied procession of events, which we call Nature, affords a sublime spectacle and an inexhaustible wealth of attractive problems to the speculative observer. If we confine our attention to that aspect which engages the attention of the intellect, nature appears a beautiful and harmonious whole, the incarnation of a faultless logical process, from certain premisses in the past to an inevitable conclusion in [the] future. But if it be regarded from a less elevated, though more human, point of view; if our moral sympathies are allowed to influence our judgment, and we permit ourselves to criticise our great mother as we criticise one another; then our verdict, at least so far as sentient nature is concerned, can hardly be so favourable. In sober truth, to those who have made a study of the phenomena of life as they exhibited by the higher forms of the animal world, the optimistic dogma, that this is the best of all possible worlds, will seem little better than a libel upon possibility. \cite[p. 195-96]{huxley1} \end{quote} Huxley went on to compare the natural world with ``a gladiator's show": \begin{quote} The creatures are [\ldots] set to fight---whereby the strongest, the swiftest, and the cunningest live to fight another day. The spectator has no need to turn his thumbs down, as no quarter is given. He must admit that the skill and training displayed are wonderful. But he must shut his eyes if he would not see that more or less enduring suffering is the meed of both vanquished and victor. And [\ldots] the great game is going on in every corner of the world, thousands of times a minute; [\ldots] were our ears sharp enough, we need not descend to the gates of hell to hear \begin{quote} sospiri, pianti, ed alti guai. \ldots Voci alte e floche, e suon di man con elle[.\footnote{This passage from Dante's \emph{Divine Comedy} translates as follows: \begin{quote} Languages diverse, horrible dialects, Accents of anger, words of agony, And voices high and hoarse, with sound of hands,\\ Made up a tumult that goes whirling on For ever in that air for ever black, Even as the sand doth, when the whirlwind breathes. \cite[Inferno: Canto III]{divinecomedy} \end{quote}}] \end{quote} \cite[p. 200]{huxley1} \end{quote} Nature doesn't care if the survivors are miserable; it only cares that they survive.\footnote{There may be some evolutionary argument for the hypothesis that, before animals mature, their lives are not brutally awful most of the time, for if their lives were, the animals might find it harder to reproduce. (In fact, this is one of many arguments against pain in plants: if plants did suffer, it would only induce stress and would yield no survival benefit, since they wouldn't be able to run away.) Once animals enter old age, though, there's no evolutionary influence to counteract terrible suffering; for instance, there's no evolutionary benefit for having a mechanism to palliate the pain of death, since such a mechanism could not enhance the organism's ability to propagate children \cite{dawkins}. In addition, this analysis ignores the vast majority of animals that \textit{don't} make it to reproductive age. These individuals may very well suffer horribly as they die from hunger, disease, and predation. Indeed, in this case, there's an evolutionary argument for the hypothesis that most animals which fail to reproduce and die young have miserable lives \cite{ng, ngkangaroo}. In many species, fitness is enhanced by overproducing offspring so that only those best adapted survive. ``Most children left behind" could be the motto of this process.} \cite{futurehuman} \section{Conclusion} %To this question, I don't have a good answer. I hope that readers more knowledgeable about the subject than I am will contact me with ideas. %Additional research into the degree of wild-animal suffering may be cost-effective. In particular, it would be helpful to have a better assessment of the probability of insect sentience. If further study changes our subjective probability from, say, 0.03 to 0.3, then the problem of wild-animal suffering will have become nearly ten times as urgent. %Also important is the question of whether it would be possible to intervene in nature to reduce animal suffering. A few philosophers have raised the question of whether such intervention is morally obligatory \cite{1, 2, 3, 4}, though as the quotations in the Introduction noted, such intervention should aim to avoid causing more harm than good. Are there or might it be possible to develop technologies that could, say, ease the painfulness of wild-animal deaths? Would such techniques be applicable to insects and other small animals that account for nearly all of the expected suffering in the wild? I look forward to investigating this topic further. The challenge of wild-animal suffering is difficult, but it is also highly important. Between 50 and 100 million animals are killed each year in laboratory experiments, according to sources in \cite{animal-testing}. A conservative figure for the number of land-based farm animals killed for food each year around the world is 55 billion, according to the UN Food and Agriculture Organization (see \cite{world-farm-animals-day}). But there are $10^{18}$ insects in the world \cite{somany}, and a few more non-insect wild animals. As serious as animal pain due to experiments and agriculture may be, the number of wild animals that suffer in preventable ways is probably far higher. %It seems that there ought to be a cost-effective way of relieving at least some of this suffering. A few philosophers have raised the question of whether intervention in nature is morally obligatory \cite{1, 2, 3, 4}. Of course, as the quotations in Section \ref{Introduction} noted, such intervention might often cause more harm than good, at least until scientists acquire a much better understanding of ecology. But perhaps some day, humans will develop the knowledge and technology to address the problem in a serious way. In order to make that day come sooner, perhaps the best thing we can do is promote concern for wild-animal suffering as a serious ethical issue, among animal-welfare activists, academics, and the general public. (See ``The Importance of Wild-Animal Suffering," \url{http://www.utilitarian-essays.com/suffering-nature.html}, for further discussion.) Little action to help wild animals will be taken until the brutality of nature is put on the map of social concerns in the same way that, e.g., inhumane animal treatment in laboratories and factory farms was over the past few decades, thanks to Peter Singer, Tom Regan, and others. If readers can think of anyone who might be interested in writing an \emph{Animal Liberation}-style defense of the urgency of wild-animal suffering, please let me know: webmaster [``at"] utilitarian-essays.com \begin{flushleft} \begin{thebibliography}{widest-label} \bibitem{lion}``African Lion." \emph{Animal Fact Sheets}. Mar. 2006. Woodland Park Zoo. 16 July 2006 (http://www.zoo.org/educate/fact\_sheets/savana/af\_lion.htm). \bibitem{animal-testing} ``Animal Testing." \emph{Wikpedia}. 25 July 2007. 25 July 2007 (http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Animal\_testing). \bibitem{world-farm-animals-day}``Annual Animal Death Toll Levels Off." \emph{World Farm Animals Day.} 3 Oct. 2006. 25 July 2007 (http://www.wfad.org/mediacenter/NRVictims6.pdf). \bibitem{availability}``Availability Heuristic." \emph{Wikipedia}. 10 Dec. 2006. 26 Dec. 2006 (http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Availability\_heuristic). \bibitem{insectnumbers}``Basic Facts: Insect Numbers." \emph{O. Orkin Insect Zoo, National Museum of Natural History, Smithsonian Institution}. 1997. Mississippi State University. 8 July 2006 (\url{http://insectzoo.msstate.edu/Students/basic.numbers.html}). \bibitem{i2}``Beneficial Insects in Your Backyard." \emph{University of Maine Cooperative Extension} Bulletin 2490. 3 April 2006. 6 July 2006 (http://www.umext.maine.edu/onlinepubs/htmpubs/habitats/7150.htm). \bibitem{i3}``Biting Insects." \emph{Doktor Doom}. 6 July 2006 (\url{http://www.doktordoom.com/biting.html}). \bibitem{futurehuman}Bostrom, Nick. ``The Future of Human Evolution." \emph{Death and Anti-Death: Two Hundred Years After Kant, Fifty Years After Turing}. Ed. Charles Tandy. Palo Alto: Ria University Press, 2004. 339-71. \emph{Nick Bostrom's Home Page}. 2004. 8 July 2006 (\url{http://www.nickbostrom.com/fut/evolution.html}). \bibitem{golden}$---$. ``Golden." \emph{Nick Bostrom's Home Page}. 2004. 29 July 2006 (\url{http://www.nickbostrom.com/fable/retriever.html}). \bibitem{nuffield2}Bourne, Debra C., Penny Cusdin, and Suzanne I. Boardman, eds. \emph{Pain Management in Ruminants}. My information was taken from a web-page description of the book: ``Pain Management in Ruminants." \emph{Wildlife Information Network}. 20 July 2006 (http://www.wildlifeinformation.org/About/Vol\_PainManagement.htm). \bibitem{squirrel}``A Brief History of the Squirrel." \emph{Squirrels.org}. 8 July 2006 (\url{http://www.squirrels.org/history.html}). \bibitem{bullfrog}``Bullfrog \emph{Rana catesbeiana}." \emph{AllAboutFrogs.org}. 8 July 2006 (\url{http://allaboutfrogs.org/info/species/bullfrog.html}). The average bullfrog lives 4 to 5 years. \bibitem{lab}``Captivity and Suffering." \emph{UCLA Animal Care and Use Training Manual}. UCLA Office for the Protection of Research Subjects. 16 July 2006 (http://www.oprs.ucla.edu/animal/help/manual/Models\_ResearchAndTeaching/\\ EthicalConsiderations\_CaptivitySuffering.asp). \bibitem{cat}``Cat Facts." \emph{CatsInfo.com}. 2001. 8 July 2006 (\url{http://www.catsinfo.com/catfacts.html}). I settled on an average lifespan of four years because this source reported that ``The average lifespan of an outdoor-only cat is about 3 to 5 years [\ldots]." \bibitem{cats}``Cats and Wildlife." \emph{Audubon Society of Portland}. 18 July 2006 (http://www.audubonportland.org/livingwithwildlife/brochures/cats\_wildlife). \bibitem{ngkangaroo}Clarke, Matthew and Yew-Kwang Ng. ``Population Dynamics and Animal Welfare: Issues Raised by the Culling of Kangaroos in Puckapunyal." Accepted for publication in \emph{Social Choice and Welfare}. \bibitem{cheetah}Conover, Adele. ``Educating Cheetah." \emph{National Wildlife} 41.5 (2003). \emph{National Wildlife Federation}. Aug./Sept. 2003. 16 July 2006 (http://www.nwf.org/nationalwildlife/article.cfm?issueID=63\&articleID=811). \bibitem{cold2}``Continued Rain, Snowpack Leaves Animals Hungry." \emph{Associated Press} 23 Apr. 2006. CBS 13/UPN 31. 16 July 2006 (\url{http://cbs13.com/topstories/local\_story\_113182048.html}). \bibitem{4}Cowen, Tyler. ``Policing Nature." \emph{Tyler Cowen's Personal Web Page}. 19 May 2001. George Mason University. 20 July 2006 (http://www.gmu.edu/jbc/Tyler/police.doc). \bibitem{coyote}``Coyote." \emph{Phoenix Zoo}. 16 July 2006 (http://www.phoenixzoo.org/learn/animals/animal\_detail.aspx?\\ FACT\_SHEET\_ID=100010). \bibitem{divinecomedy}Dante. \emph{Divine Comedy}. \emph{Italian Language, About.com}. 20 July 2006 (http://italian.about.com/library/anthology/dante/blinferno003.htm). \bibitem{heron}Davis, Tom. ``Lava Heron." \emph{Home Page -- Tom Davis}. 29 June 2001. 16 July 2006 (\url{http://www.geometer.org/gal01/heron/index.html}). \bibitem{dawkins}Dawkins, Richard. ``Article in \emph{The Telegraph} Wednesday May 10th, 1995." \emph{The Telegraph} 10 May 1995. 18 July 2006 (\url{http://www.simonyi.ox.ac.uk/dawkins/WorldOfDawkins-archive/Dawkins/Work/Articles/1995-05-10nomercy.shtml}). Adapted from Dawkins, Richard. \emph{River Out of Eden: A Darwinian View of Life}. HarperCollins Publishers, 1996. \bibitem{depressive} ``Depressive Realism." \emph{Wikipedia}. 17 July 2006. 30 July 2006 (http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Depressive\_realism). \bibitem{deer}Dewey, T. and Animal Diversity Web Staff. ``\emph{Odocoileus virginianus}." \emph{Animal Diversity Web}. 2003. University of Michigan Museum of Zoology. 8 July 2006 (http://animaldiversity.ummz.umich.edu/site/accounts/information/\\ Odocoileus\_virginianus.html). \bibitem{ptsd}El-Hage, Wissam and Catherine Belzung. ``Unavoidable Predatory Stress in Mice: An Animal Model of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder." \emph{Universite Francois-Rabelais, Tours}. 2002. 20 July 2006 (http://www.univ-tours.fr/ed/elhage.pdf). \bibitem{bambi}Everett, Jennifer. ``Environmental Ethics, Animal Welfarism, and the Problem of Predation: A Bambi Lover's Respect for Nature." \emph{Ethics and the Environment} 6.1 (2001): 42-67. \bibitem{torturerack}``Fierce Ants Build `Torture Rack.'" \emph{BBC News} 23 April 2005. 16 July 2006 (http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/science/nature/4472521.stm). \bibitem{3}Fink, Charles K. ``The Predation Argument." \emph{Between the Species} 5 (2005). California Polytechnic State University. 4 July 2006 (\url{http://cla.calpoly.edu/~jlynch/fink.html}). \bibitem{flank}Flank, Lenny. ``Live Prey vs. Prekill." \emph{The Snake: An Owner's Guide To A Happy Healthy Pet}. Howell Book House, 1997. Accessed from \emph{Melissa Kaplan's Herp Care Collection}. 17 Aug. 2002. 16 July 2006 (\url{http://www.anapsid.org/prekill2.html}). \bibitem{albert}Free, Ann Cottrell. \emph{Animals, Nature, and Albert Schweitzer}. The Albert Schweitzer Fellowship, The Albert Schweitzer Center, The Animal Welfare Institute, and The Humane Society of the United States, 1982. \emph{Animal Welfare Institute} 2002. 4 July 2006 (http://www.awionline.org/schweitzer/as-3.htm). \bibitem{snake}``Garter Snake (\emph{Thamnophis sirtalis})." \emph{Animal Planet}. Discovery Channel. 8 July 2006 (\url{http://animal.discovery.com/fansites/jeffcorwin/carnival/slithering/garter.html}). The average lifespan of a garter snake may be around 2 years. \bibitem{thinktank}``Good Question\ldots: \ldots Does the Savagery of Predation in Nature Show That God Either Isn't, or at Least Isn't Good-Hearted?" \emph{A Christian Thinktank}. 18 July 1999. 8 July 2006 (\url{http://www.christian-thinktank.com/predator.html}). \bibitem{gould}Gould, Stephen Jay. ``Nonmoral Nature." \textit{Hen's Teeth and Horse's Toes: Further Reflections in Natural History}. New York: W. W. Norton, 1994. pp. 32-44. \textit{Unofficial Stephen Jay Gould Archive}. 8 June 2006 (\url{http://www.stephenjaygould.org/library/gould\_nonmoral.html}). \bibitem{cold1}Heidorn, Keith C. ``Ice Storms: Hazardous Beauty." Dec. 2001. 16 July 2006 (http://www.islandnet.com/~see/weather/elements/icestorm.htm). \bibitem{birds}Hounsome, Mike V. ``Lifespan of Birds." 2003. Offwell Woodland \& Wildlife Trust. 8 July 2006 (http://www.offwell.free-online.co.uk/bird\_lifespan.htm). \bibitem{humanehunt}``Hunting: Kindness or Cruelty?" \emph{Conservation} 8.3 (1984). \emph{Nova Scotia Department of Natural Resources}. 16 July 2006 (http://www.gov.ns.ca/natr/wildlife/conserva/08-03-1.htm). \bibitem{fishy}Huntingford, F. A., C. Adams, V. A. Braithwaite, S. Kadri, T. G. Pottinger, P. Sandoe, and J. F. Turnbull. ``Current Issues in Fish Welfare." \emph{Journal of Fish Biology} 68.2 (2006): 332-72. \bibitem{huxley1}Huxley, Thomas. ``The Struggle for Existence in Human Society [1888]." \emph{Evolution \& Ethics and Other Essays}. 1894. 195-236. Charles Blinderman and David Joyce. \emph{The Huxley File}. Clark College. 20 July 2006 (\url{http://aleph0.clarku.edu/huxley/CE9/str.html}). \bibitem{i5}``Insect Order Information." \emph{Bugscope}. Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. 6 July 2006 (http://bugscope.beckman.uiuc.edu/resources/insects/insectorders.htm). \bibitem{rats}Jackson, W. B. ``Norway Rat and Allies." \emph{Wild Mammals of North America}. Ed. J. A. Chapman and G. A. Feldhamer. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press, 1982. 1077-88. \bibitem{james}James, William. \emph{The Varieties of Religious Experience: A Study in Human Nature}. Electronic Text Center, University of Virginia Library. (\url{http://etext.virginia.edu/toc/modeng/public/JamVari.html}). \bibitem{kruuk}Kruuk, H. \emph{The Spotted Hyena}. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1972. \bibitem{lack}Lack, D. \emph{The Natural Regulation of Animal Numbers}. Oxford University Press, 1954. \bibitem{honey-bees}Lewis, Noah. ``Bees Feel Pain." \emph{Vegetus.org}. 25 July 2007 (http://www.vegetus.org/honey/pain.htm). \bibitem{spiders}Lin, Lorraine, Sophie Huxley, and colleagues. ``Spiders, Silks and Webs." \emph{Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council}. 16 July 2006 (\url{http://www.bbsrc.ac.uk/society/engagement/schools/resources/spiders/spiders.html}). \bibitem{lockwood}Lockwood, Jeffrey A. ``The Moral Standing of Insects and the Ethics of Extinction." \emph{The Florida Entomologist} 70.1 (1987): 70-89. \bibitem{mal}``Malnutrition and Starvation." \emph{Michigan Department of Natural Resources}. 16 July 2006 (\url{http://www.michigan.gov/dnr/0,1607,7-153-10370\_12150\_12220-26946--,00.html}). \bibitem{cold3}Mastalir, Linda. ``Long Winter Creates Stress for Wild Animals." \emph{Radio Prague} 9 Mar. 2006. 16 July 2006 (http://www.radio.cz/en/article/76658). \bibitem{raptor}McGowan, Christopher. \emph{The Raptor and the Lamb: Predators and Prey in the Living World}. New York: Henry Holt and Company, 1997. \bibitem{raccoon}Messmer, Terry A., Robert W. Seabloom, Richard D. Crawford, Karen L. Kriel, and Ron A. Stromstad. ``Raccoon (\emph{Procyon lotor})." University of North Dakota. 8 July 2006 (\url{http://www.und.nodak.edu/org/ndwild/raccoon.html}). \bibitem{dogs}Mott, Maryann. ``U.S. Facing Feral-Dog Crisis [page 2]." \emph{National Geographic News} 21 August 2003. National Geographic Society. 8 July 2006 (\url{http://news.nationalgeographic.com/news/2003/08/0821\_030821\_straydogs\_2.html}). I assume here that most dogs killed on roads are feral dogs. \bibitem{2}Naess, Arne. ``Should We Try To Relieve Clear Cases of Extreme Suffering in Nature?" \emph{Pan Ecology} 6.1 (1991). \bibitem{hindu}Nair, R. Madhavan. ``Hunger and Thirst Haunt Wildlife." \emph{The Hindu} 26 Mar. 2004. \emph{hindu.com}. 16 July 2006 (http://www.hindu.com/2004/03/26/stories/2004032608110500.htm). \bibitem{ng}Ng, Yew-Kwang. ``Towards Welfare Biology: Evolutionary Economics of Animal Consciousness and Suffering." \emph{Biology and Philosophy} 10.4 (1995): 255-85. \bibitem{nuffield}Nuffield Council on Bioethics. \emph{Ethics of Research Involving Animals}. 20 July 2006 (\url{http://http://www.nuffieldbioethics.org/go/browseablepublications/\\ethicsofresearchanimals/report\_230.html}). \bibitem{opossum}``Opossum General Information." \emph{Opossum Society of the United States}. 2003. 8 July 2006 (\url{http://www.opossumsocietyus.org/opossum.html}). \bibitem{i4}``Order Diptera." \emph{Oklahoma State University Center for Veterinary Health Sciences}. 6 July 2006 (http://www.cvm.okstate.edu/instruction/kocan/vpar5333/533ot5aa.htm). \bibitem{1984}Orwell, George. \emph{1984}. 1949. ``George Orwell's 1984." \emph{msxnet.org: Sean Young's Website}. 8 July 2006 (http://www.msxnet.org/orwell/1984). \bibitem{howstuff}Perry, Lacy. ``How Snakes Work: Feeding." \emph{howstuffworks}. 16 July 2006 (http://science.howstuffworks.com/snake4.htm). \bibitem{piranha}``Piranha." \emph{Extreme Science}. 2005. 16 July 2006 (http://www.extremescience.com/Piranha.htm). \bibitem{reserve}``Project `Reservation.'" \emph{utilitarian.org}. 1999. 4 July 2006 (\url{http://utilitarian.org/projects/reservation.html}). \bibitem{worm1}``Proventricular or Stomach Worm." \emph{Michigan Department of Natural Resources}. 16 July 2006 (\url{http://www.michigan.gov/dnr/0,1607,7-153-10370\_12150\_12220-27255--,00.html}). \bibitem{disease1}``Rabies." \emph{Michigan Department of Natural Resources}. 16 July 2006 (\url{http://www.michigan.gov/dnr/0,1607,7-153-10370\_12150\_12220-27259--,00.html}). \bibitem{ecopreview}Rigby, Kate. ``Minding (about) Matter: On the Eros and Anguish of Earthly Encounter." \emph{Australian Humanities Review} 28 (2006). \emph{Library - La Trobe University}. 16 July 2006 (\url{http://www.lib.latrobe.edu.au/AHR/archive/Issue-April-2006/EcoRigby.html}). \bibitem{rosy}``Rosy Retrospection." \emph{Wikipedia}. 29 Nov. 2006. 26 Dec. 2006 (http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Rosy\_retrospection). \bibitem{worms2}``Roundworms." \emph{Michigan Department of Natural Resources}. 16 July 2006 (\url{http://www.michigan.gov/dnr/0,1607,7-153-10370\_12150\_12220-27263--,00.html}). \bibitem{disease2}``Salmonellosis." \emph{Michigan Department of Natural Resources}. 16 July 2006 (\url{http://www.michigan.gov/dnr/0,1607,7-153-10370\_12150\_12220-27268--,00.html}). \bibitem{1}Sapontzis, Steve F. ``Predation." \emph{Ethics and Animals} 5.2 (1984): 27-38. \bibitem{schaller}Schaller, G. B. \emph{The Serengeti Lion}. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1972. \bibitem{selection}``Selection Bias." \emph{Wikipedia}. 16 Dec. 2006. 26 Dec. 2006 (http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Selection\_bias). \bibitem{british}``Session Four - Animal Welfare; Drag and Bloodhound Hunting; Implementing a Ban." \emph{Oral Evidence Sessions, Committee of Inquiry into Hunting with Dogs in England and Wales}. 10 April 2000. 18 July 2006 (http://www.huntinginquiry.gov.uk/mainsections/oralevidence/oral10aprilsess4.htm). \bibitem{shakespearebug}Shakespeare, William. \emph{Measure for Measure}. 1603. \emph{The Free Library}. 20 July 2006 (http://shakespeare.thefreelibrary.com/Measure-for-Measure). \bibitem{singer}Singer, Peter. ``Food for Thought." [Reply to a letter by David Rosinger.] \emph{New York Review of Books} 20.10 (1973). \emph{NYBooks.com}. 4 July 2006 (http://www.nybooks.com/articles/9822). \bibitem{insectpain}Smith, Jane A. ``A Question of Pain in Invertebrates." \emph{ILAR Journal} 33.1-2 (1991). 6 July 2006 (\url{http://dels.nas.edu/ilar\_n/ilarjournal/33\_1\_2/V33\_1\_2Question.shtml}). \bibitem{popbook}Solbrig, O. T. and D. J. Solbrig. \emph{Introduction to Population Biology and Evolution}. London: Addison-Wesley, 1979. \bibitem{somany}``So Many Insects." \emph{The Manduca Project}. 14 Oct. 2003. The University of Arizona. 12 July 2006 (\url{http://insected.arizona.edu/manduca/ins\_many.html}). \bibitem{boa}``Squeeze and Serve." \emph{Eaten Alive - The World of Predators}. Scitech Online. 16 July 2006 (http://www.scitech.org.au/eatenalive/download/exhibits/ea\_squeeze\_and\_serve.pdf). \bibitem{i1}Sullivan, Mark. ``Research on the Fly: Biologist O'Connor Says the Little Drosophila May Tell Us a Lot about the Human Aging Process." \emph{Boston College Chronicle} 10.11 (2002). Boston College. 6 July 2006 (\url{http://www.bc.edu/bc\_org/rvp/pubaf/chronicle/v10/f14/oconnor.html}). \bibitem{disease3}``Verminous Hemorrhagic Ulcerative Enteritis." \emph{Michigan Department of Natural Resources}. 16 July 2006 (\url{http://www.michigan.gov/dnr/0,1607,7-153-10370\_12150\_12220-27317--,00.html}). \bibitem{wallace}Wallace, Alfred Russel. ``Is Nature Cruel? The Purpose and Limitations of Pain." \emph{The World of Life}. 1910. Charles H. Smith. \emph{The Alfred Russel Wallace Page}. 25 May 2006. Western Kentucky University. 20 July 2006 (http://www.wku.edu/~smithch/wallace/S732CH19.htm). \bibitem{weberlaw}``Weber$-$Fechner Law." \emph{Wikipedia}. 27 Dec. 2006. 27 Dec. 2006 (http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Weber-Fechner\_law). \bibitem{farmerfriend}``Why is the Ichneumon Fly Known as the Farmer's Friends?" \emph{4to40.com}. 26 Dec. 2006 (http://www.4to40.com/qa/index.asp?counter=160\&category=animal). \bibitem{wishful}``Wishful Thinking." \emph{Wikipedia}. 15 Dec. 2006. 26 Dec. 2006 (http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Wishful\_thinking). \bibitem{word}Wordsworth, William. ``Animal Tranquility and Decay." 1798. \emph{EveryPoet.com}. 8 July 2006 (http://www.everypoet.com/archive/poetry/William\_Wordsworth/\\ william\_wordsworth\_140.htm). \bibitem{snakey}Zimmer, Carl. ``Dangerous Curves." \emph{Natural History} Nov. 2000. \emph{CarlZimmer.com}. 16 July 2006 (\url{http://www.carlzimmer.com/articles/2000/articles\_2000\_snakes.html}). \end{thebibliography} \end{flushleft} \end{document} \textbf{Illustration 2:} Inspired by Stevens' Power Law \cite{stevens}, we might also try the following functions: $E(c) = S(c) = kc^a$ for $c \geq 0$, where $k$ and $a$ are constants such that $k > 0$ and $0 < a < 1$. . \begin{quote} Insects are capable of high rates of reproduction. Curtis Sabrosky provided an example in 1952 of the reproductive potential in house flies. Beginning with one pair of house flies in April, there would be a total of 191,000,000,000,000,000,000 flies by August if all the descendants of this pair lived and reproduced normally. Glenn Herrick found that the cabbage aphid had an average of 41 offspring per female and that the aphid had 16 generations from April to October. If all the descendants of one female aphid lived, there would be 1,560,000,000,000,000,000,000,000 aphids by the end of the summer. \cite{insectnumbers} \end{quote}